Omega-3 and Dementia

A new study has found that older people with less omega-3 in their blood are more likely to suffer from dementia. The study involved about 1000 persons 65 or older randomly sampled from two Italian towns. They were given mental tests and divided into three groups: no cognitive impairment; cognitive impairment but not demented; and demented. In addition, their blood was measured. Worse mental function was more strongly associated with total omega-3 fatty acids (p = .01) than any of the other fatty acid measures.

One more reason to think that consuming more omega-3 might improve your brain function.

Autism Linked to Mood Disorders

Mood disorders appear to be much more common among the relatives of autistic children than among the relatives of other children. A survey article about this appeared in 2004. Here is a bit of the data:

In North Carolina, between 1988 and 1990, we studied 40 autistic individuals (20 attributable to known neurological disease and 20 idiopathic). Family histories, using the family history method, without knowledge of the neurological status, showed a low incidence of major mood disorder in the neurological patients (only two had family members with major depression, none with bipolar disorder). In the idiopathic autistic patients, by contrast, major depression was found in 14 and bipolar disorder in 8 of twenty families.

Between 1995 and 2002, we acquired another series of patients included in our study of fluoxetine treatment for young autistic spectrum children. We determined family history data as before and sought information about family members with special intellectual abilities or attainments, inspired by observing such individuals in many of the families. The abilities most often were scientific, mathematical, or computational but included others (e.g., professor of philosophy, professional musician). Analysis revealed a strong correlation among three groups: autistic probands responding to fluoxetine, family members with major mood disorder (especially bipolar disorder), and family members with special intellectual abilities. In this study, history of major mood disorder (in first- and second-degree parental relatives) was assessed in 151 families. One hundred and eleven families (74%) had a history of major depression (in 102) and/or bipolar disorder (in 52).

In other words, mood disorders were more common among the relatives of autistic children who responded to fluoxetine (Prozac) than among the relatives of autistic children who did not respond to fluoxetine. I have wondered why autism seems to be increasing. This linkage suggests it may have something to do with the long-term increase in depression.

Thanks to A Room of One’s Own.

Should Mark Twain Have Won a Nobel Prize?

Of course. He didn’t. And dozens of writers you have never heard of, much less read, much less quoted in everyday conversation, have. These wrongs are corrected in an alternative universe described here. The Biology/Medicine Prize has also been fairly ridiculous, although at least Robert Gallo hasn’t gotten one:

Mistakes of commission: 1. Frontal lobotomies. 2. Eric Kandel. If you think he deserved it, read Explorers of the Black Box.

Mistakes of omission: 1. The scientists who discovered that smoking causes lung cancer. 2. The scientists who discovered that folate deficiency causes birth defects.

Several years ago, at a big Thanksgiving dinner in an Oakland loft, I told the woman sitting next to me, a genetic counselor, what a travesty the Biology prizes were. The discovery that smoking causes lung cancer had improved the lives of millions of people, I said; the discovery of so-called oncogenes hadn’t improved the life of even one person. She replied that she was the sister of one of the oncogene discoverers. The next day I learned she complained I had been rude!

The Silent Spring of Marching Bands

Rachel Carson’s Silent Spring, about the damage done by pollution, “is widely credited with launching the environmental movement in the West” (Wikipedia). Along similar but narrower lines, last week’s USA Today had an article by Joyce Cohen about hearing damage caused by being in a marching band. It begins:

There’s no bigger booster of his marching band than Mark Claffey. “I am a total band nerd!” declares Claffey, a drummer for the Golden Falcons at Franklin Heights High School in Columbus, Ohio.

There’s just one downside. At age 17, he has painful ear damage.

He says that, after indoor rehearsals, his ears started hurting, then ringing.

Now, he’s abnormally sensitive to sound. If someone cranks the car radio, “I get a sharp shooting pain in my right ear,” says Claffey. . . .

It’s the dirty little secret of the halftime show: Marching band . . . can cause irreparable hearing damage, according to Brian Fligor, director of diagnostic audiology at Children’s Hospital in Boston.

The director of a professional group of music teachers claimed that knowledge of this problem is fairly new. That’s absurd, Joyce said. Stories about hearing problems among musicians have been published in medical and professional journals for at least two decades. Music teachers don’t acknowledge their own hearing problems, several experts told her, because doing so could endanger their livelihoods. Band parents, known for their fanaticism, were sometimes dismissive. They claimed that pain and ringing in the ears are normal.

The Indianapolis Star, published by Gannett, which also owns USA Today, reprinted the article. On the newspaper’s forums, readers started a debate about whether there should be laws to protect students’ hearing.

Why Do We Like Warm Food?

Yesterday I cooked some chicken. Today I reheated the leftovers. While eating them, I had a gruesome thought: Warm food is more pleasant than food at room temperature. Could the evolutionary reason be that it is better to eat freshly-killed meat (warm) than meat killed yesterday (room temperature)? Or did a preference for warm food evolve because it caused us to prefer cooked food (sterilized) to uncooked food (unsterilized)?

Sure, thermoregulation is involved. We like warm food more when we’re cold; we like cold food more when we’re hot. Michel Cabanac has done brilliant experiments about our changing preference for hot and cold environments. But there is an overall preference for warm food. We like warm food even when we’re not cold.

In spite of thousands of books and articles promoting this or that “natural” diet, it has been incredibly hard to determine what our ancient ancestors ate, the diet that presumably fits us best. One way has been to ask what modern-day hunter-gatherers eat. Not only do their diets vary widely but also they are clearly not typical: They live in meager environments. So that is hopeless, although Weston Price showed that there was a lot to be learned by studying earlier foodways. Price was surprised to find how much those ancient foodways differed from each other yet all produced good health.

The most basic questions about our ancient diet remain unanswered. Did our ancestors eat lots of meat (savannah evolution) or lots of fish (aquatic ape theory) or neither (vegetarian proponents)? In spite of looking, Price never found a group that ate little meat that was in the best health, so I doubt the vegetarians. I suspect ancient peoples ate lots of fish at first and then started eating lots of meat as they spread away from the coasts. My main evidence for the fish is my omega-3 results that imply our brains work best with lots of omega-3. My main evidence for the meat is the huge popularity among boys of video games that contain elements of hunting. It’s hardly great evidence, of course, since the popularity of those games, and of actual hunting, has other plausible explanations.

This is why my omega-3 self-experimentation interests me so much. It is a way to figure out the best diet for our brain. It relies on fast simple cheap easy-to-control experiments that anyone can do, rather than on epidemiology (correlations) or expensive slow hard-to-control clinical trials that often involve unusual people.

Memorial University Defends the Indefensible

In 1993, Marilyn Harvey, who at the time was Ranjit Chandra’s research assistant, came forward to say that a paper by Chandra reported research that didn’t happen. Memorial University conducted an investigation that failed to confirm her (very courageous) allegation. About that investigation, Ranjit Chandra’s Wikipedia entry says the following:

The vice-presidents were unable to secure the data, and, as a consequence, were unable to verify research fraud conclusively.

Huh? Harvey’s claim was that the data didn’t exist!

This sentence was written by Peter S. Morris, Director of Public Affairs at Memorial. I emailed Morris to try to find out how it could make sense. Presumably it made sense to Morris. Alas, Morris would not explain it. He did say that to prove research fraud — in Chandra’s case, the fraud of making up data — you need the data. You read that correctly: To prove that someone has made up data you need to have the data, Morris asserted. He wouldn’t explain that, either.

Memorial’s behavior did great harm to Marilyn Harvey, as you can read in the complaint filed with her lawsuit.

Interview with Andy Maul about Test Development (part 3)

5. What are you doing to develop a better test?

Not being a content expert in either emotions or intelligence myself, I have no plans at the moment to create a test of emotional intelligence. Instead, my goal is to explore and discuss, firstly, better ways of engaging in the iterative process of construct exploration and test development, and secondly, better methods of test analysis. These are, of course, interrelated.

The “classical” method of test construction in psychology goes like this: a) decide what you want to measure (formally or informally); b) write items to measure it; c) pilot those items; d) run basic statistical analyses, such as Cronbach’s alpha e) remove the items from the test that are the least reliable with the other items, thus improving the reliability of the test, and f) publish.

This process usually yields a reasonably reliable test. A problem with this approach is that nowhere did we allow the
process of test construction to inform our theory development. Test construction can be as much a process of construct exploration as anything else, if we allow it. For instance, think-alouds and exit interviews can help us understand what subjects are actually thinking as they take the test, and whether the variation in the ways people approach the items truly reflects variation in the construct we think we’re measuring. The exercise of construct mapping can turn a murky idea of what we’re measuring into a much clearer one, by laying out a priori theories about what kinds of items measure what levels of the construct, and those ideas can then be empirically tested later, which makes the analysis phase much more informative than it traditionally is. And, of course, I would be remiss if I didn’t mention the analysis itself: item response modeling often affords valuable information missed by classical analysis, such as information on person and item “fit” (which, to be interpretable, requires going back to the theory of the link between the items and the construct itself, which once again benefits from thoughtful construct mapping) and information about dimensionality at the item level (as opposed to the branch level, which is where confirmatory factor models—such as the ones used to investigate the structure of the emotional intelligence tests I’m working with—traditionally concentrate).

Doing things in this manner usually takes more than one iteration, which is one reason people might not like it. So far, the MSCEIT has been developed and evaluated, and the test developers have spent a good deal of time debating other authors in the literature concerning the value of the test, but the analyses have not yet led to test revision (except in the manner I described above: that items with poor reliability were dropped, without any particular theory about *why* they were unreliable).

So, in other words: I won’t claim to be a substance expert enough to be able to write a new test of emotional intelligence on my own, but I would like to use the measurement efforts in this field as a way to discuss construct exploration and instrument development in psychological research.

Part 1. Part 2.

Reference

Wilson, M. (2005) Constructing Measures: An Item Response Modeling Approach. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, New Jersey.

Marilyn Harvey’s Complaint

In 1993, Marilyn Harvey, a nurse, complained to Memorial University that her boss, world-famous Order-of-Canada scientist Dr. Ranjit Chandra, could not have done the research he claimed. A very courageous thing to do. After an investigation, Memorial did not agree. Harvey recently sued Memorial. From her complaint:

The Plaintiff [Harvey] says that the Defendant [Memorial] defamed her by taking actions which . . . caused her to be isolated, shunned, and humiliated through the following:
(a) representing to the community that her complaint was unjustified;
(b) misconducting the investigation of the complaint;
(c) misleading the research community as to the reasons for discontinuing the investigation;
(d) choosing not to conduct another investigation;
(e) misleading the Plaintiff as to the reasons for discontinuing the investigation;
(f) acquiescing in and adopting [?] the actions of Dr. Chandra when he sued her for theft of research data; and by its conduct giving the Plaintiff and the public the impression that it believed the allegations of theft to be true;
(g) treating the Plaintiff in a manner as to imply to her and the university and the healthcare communities, and the public, that her complaint was unjustified;
(h) acquiescing in and adopting statements of Dr. Chandra which impugned the Plaintiff’s motives and integrity.
The overall effect of the conduct of the Defendant was to constitute a communication to the community, and to the research and hospital community in particular, that was profoundly defamatory. . . It expose[d] her to contempt, ridicule and marginalization and [caused her] to be viewed by co-workers as a troublemaker and a pariah who could have a detrimental effect on one’s career if she were not avoided.

Memorial’s defense.