An Endangered Language

This is the most moving YouTube video I have ever seen.

Background here.

Several years ago I visited Alaska with a friend. We stayed in Juno. One day I visited a nearby glacier. A visitor’s center had a slide show about the glacier, with taped narration from a park ranger. The glacier came out in the winter, he said, and retreated during the summer. He spoke about plants and animals nearby. It was all very factual and flat but you could tell the speaker cared a lot about the glacier. How rare, I thought. Not the emotion — people care about lots of things — but its expression.

Something is Better Than Nothing

I have been asked to write six columns about common scientific mistakes for the journal Nutrition. This is a draft of the first. I am very interested in feedback, especially about what you don’t like.

Lesson 1. Doing something is better than doing nothing.

“You should go to the studio everyday,” a University of Michigan art professor named Richard Sears told his students. “There’s no guarantee that you’ll make something good — but if you don’t go, you’re guaranteed to make nothing.” The same is true of science. Every research plan has flaws, often big ones — but if you don’t do anything, you won’t learn anything.

I have been asked to write six columns about common scientific mistakes. The mistakes I see are mostly mistakes of omission.

A few years ago I visited a pediatrician in Stockholm. She was interested in the connection between sunlight and illness (children are much healthier in the summer) and had been considering doing a simple correlational study. When she told her colleagues about it, they said: Your study doesn’t control for X. You should a more difficult study. It was awful advice. In the end, she did nothing.

Science is all about learning from experience. It is a kind of fancy trial and error. But this modest description is not enough for some scientists, who create rules about proper behavior. Rule 1. You must do X (e.g., double-blind placebo-controlled experiments). Rule 2. You must not do Y (e.g., “uncontrolled” experiments). Such ritualistic thinking is common in scientific discussions, hurting not only the discussants — it makes them dismissive — but also those they might help. Sure, some experimental designs are better than others. It’s the overstatement, the notion that experiments in a certain group are not worth doing, that is the problem. It is likely that the forbidden experiments, whatever their flaws, are better than nothing. A group that has suffered from this way of thinking is people with bipolar disorder. Over the last thirty years, few new treatments for this problem have been developed. According to Post and Luckenbaugh (2003, p. 71), “many of us in the academic community have inadvertently participated in the limitation of a generation of research on bipolar illness . . . by demands for methodological purity or study comprehensiveness that can rarely be achieved.”

Rituals have right and wrong. Science is more practical. The statistician John Tukey wrote about ritualistic thinking among psychologists in an article called “Analyzing data: Sanctification or detective work?” (Tukey, 1969). One of his examples involved measurement typology. The philosopher of science N. R. Campbell had come up with the notion, popularized by Stevens (1946), that scales of measurement could be divided into four types: ratio, interval, ordinal, and nominal. Weight and age are ratio scales, for example; rating how hungry you are is an ordinal measure. The problem, said Tukey, were the accompanying prohibitions. Campbell said you can add two measurements (e.g., two heights) only if the scale is ratio or interval; if you are dealing with ordinal or nominal measures, you cannot. The effect of such prohibitions, said Tukey, is to make it less likely that you will learn something you could have learned. (See Velleman and Wilkinson, 1993, for more about what’s wrong with this typology.)

I fell victim to right-and-wrong thinking as a graduate student. I had started to use a new way to study timing and had collected data from ten rats. I plotted the data from each rat separately and looked at the ten graphs. I did not plot the average of the rats because I had read an article about how, with data like mine, averages can be misleading — they can show something not in any of the data being averaged. For example, if you average bimodal distributions you may get a unimodal distribution and vice-versa. After several months, however, I averaged my data anyway; I can’t remember why. Looking at the average, I immediately noticed a feature of the data (symmetry) that I hadn’t noticed when looking at each rat separately. The symmetry was important (Roberts, 1981).

A corollary is this: If someone (else) did something, they probably learned something. And you can probably learn something from what they did. For a few years, I attended a meeting called Animal Behavior Lunch where we discussed new animal behavior articles. All of the meetings consisted of graduate students talking at great length about the flaws of that week’s paper. The professors in attendance knew better but somehow we did not manage to teach this. The students seemed to have a very strong bias to criticize. Perhaps they had been told that “critical thinking” is good. They may have never been told that appreciation should come first. I suspect failure to teach graduate students to see clearly the virtues of flawed research is the beginning of the problem I discuss here: Mature researchers who don’t do this or that because they have been told not to do it (it is “flawed”) and as a result do nothing.

References

Post RM, Luckenbaugh DA.. Unique design issues in clinical trials of patients with bipolar affective disorder. J Psychiatr Res. 2003 Jan-Feb;37(1):61-73.

Roberts, S. (1981). Isolation of an internal clock. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 7, 242-268.

Stevens, S.S. (1946). On the theory of scales of measurement. Science, 103, 677-680.

Tukey, J. W. (1969). Analyzing data: Sanctification or detective work. American Psychologist, 24, 83-91.

Velleman PF, Wilkinson L. Nominal, Ordinal, Interval, and Ratio Typologies Are Misleading. The American Statistician, Vol. 47, No. 1. (1993), pp. 65-72.

SLD Phenomenology (part 3)

Kris from South Carolina writes:

I have an interesting side effect of the SLD diet: I have not peeled from sunburn. I have been using the extra light olive oil. I have light skin and usually peel even after a minor sunburn. It has not happened since I have been taking the oil. I tested it last week at the beach. Several hours on the beach with no sunscreen. I got a little red but after a week–no peeling. I’ve also lost 25 lbs in a couple of months.

Part 1. Part 2.

More Self-Congratulation (probably erroneous)

July 2, 2007. Online issue of The New Yorker appears. Editorial by Hertzberg is based on a series of articles in the Washington Post, but does not link to them.

July 2, 2007. I write to the New Yorker webmaster: If an article refers to something Web-available, why not link to it?

July 9, 2007. New Financial Page appears. Number of links: 7. Number of links in previous three Financial Pages: 0.

Earlier self-congratulation.

Why is Sicko So Good?

In What is Art? Tolstoy argued that the goal of art is to evoke emotion. According to a Blue Cross vice president, “You would have to be dead to be unaffected by [Sicko].”

Why is Sicko so good?

I have a theory: the Internet. At his website, Moore asked for health-insurance horror stories. He got 25 thousand submissions, I have read. With that much to work with, you can select some extremely moving stories. Not only that. In an article I wrote for Spy, my editor crossed out some comment I had made. “Sometimes the material is so good it speaks for itself,” she said. Moore’s material was so good it spoke for itself. Because Moore said less the diversity of voices was increased, a big artistic plus.

There is a connection with self-experimentation. I was surprised how effective my self-experimentation turned out to be — effective scientifically. Far more than my other research (just as Sicko is far better than Moore’s other movies). I came to believe that there was a large plodding element in effective science — to find new cause-effect relationships, you needed to be able to try lots of things. Self-experimentation worked so well because it made it easy to plod, to try lots of things. Sicko is so good because his website made it easy for Moore to gather lots of good stories.

Moore and Jane Jacobs.

Advances in Retailing: Penny-Free Store

Alko Office Supply, a small Berkeley store, is penny free: If you pay cash, prices are rounded down to the nearest nickel. I have never seen this anywhere else nor had the store clerk I asked. They thought of it themselves, she said. Farmer’s market prices are always rounded down to the nickel or dime, in my experience. Here and there I have made small credit card purchases (< $20) with no signature required.

Addendum: Wikipedia entry on penny elimination. A penny protester.